4. Selforganization in the Brain
4.3 Function
Brain function can be studied at a number of different levels and with
several techniques. It is not at all clear which approach will be most
suitable for understanding higher brain functions. Electroencephalogram
(EEG) recordings give information on the averaged electrical activity in a
volume of cortical brain tissue, containing a large number of neurons. EEG
records generally have a noise-like appearance indicating an ongoing erratic
activity, which only seems to disappear in very deep anesthesia. This
activity is aperiodic, although in certain conditions (e.g. during
relaxation) it appears to be more 'rhythmic'. These rhythms, which have a
specific dominant frequency and waveform, are used to classify the EEG. In
the frequency domain, the EEG is characterized by a broad power
spectrum,
with a
continuous distribution of power
in the
frequency range 0.5-100 Hz. When rhythmic components are present,
they produce peaks in the spectrum, which are sitting on top of a broad
background. Where does this
noise-like background
activity come
from?
Isolated neurons behave deterministically: pacemaker neurons fire
action potentials periodically, while other neurons are silent unless
they are synaptically activated. Usually a number of more or less
simultaneous excitatory inputs are needed to cause such a neuron to file
(Abeles, 1982). The response of a neuron to a specific synaptic input is
predictable, because spontaneous fluctuations in membrane potential are
small compared to the depolarization needed to reach the firing threshold.
Still, when functioning in a neural network, the behaviour of neurons appears
stochastic. This activity is caused by interactions in the network, as it
disappears after blocking all synaptic transmission.
There is no noise source in the brain, as isolated neurons seem to operate
deterministically. So, how can the noise-like character of the EEG be
explained? The situation could be analogous to a gas, where the thermal
movement of the molecules seems
very erratic,
although their
movement is
completely determined by
physical laws. It is the lack of knowledge of all initial conditions, which
forces a stochastic description. When their behaviour is averaged,
deterministic macroscopic
behaviour again
appears. Only
on the
intermediate mesoscopic level stochastic behaviour dominates. This seems not to be the
case in the brain, as a noise-like
behaviour persists even on
the macroscopic
level of
EEG measurements. The
reason for this persistence is a large degree of coherence in the activity,
which also becomes apparent when comparing EEG traces from different brain
regions measured simultaneously. Although every individual trace has a
noiselike appearance, there is often a great similarity between the
traces. This spatial coherence is indicative of deterministic dynamics and
it is therefore suggestive to explain the background EEG as chaotic
activity, instead of random noise. Widespread spatial coherence is the most
important argument used by
Freeman
(1985) to explain the background EEG activity in the rabbit olfactory bulb
as chaotic. Chaotic dynamics as a basis for the EEG explains the broad power
spectrum, without the need to assume an unknown noise source.
It originates from the interplay of two conflicting forces:
destabilizing positive feedback mechanisms (mutual inhibition and recurrent
excitation) and stabilizing negative feedback. What results is an
ever-changing macroscopic activity, which is very robust, because the
dynamics are governed by a ('strange') attractor. The various rhythms
mentioned above correspond to different chaotic attractors,
probably with a relatively low fractal dimension. Such
low-dimensional attractors are well known. An example is the
Rossler-attractor, described by a set of three coupled nonlinear
differential equations. For certain parameter settings, the solution is
'chaotic, but nevertheless it has a very 'rhythmic' appearance.
Transitions between various attractors ("modes of behaviour') are governed by
control parameters. The brain is a hierarchical system, made of many
subsystems controlling each other. This implies that the output
variable of one subsystem
can act as a control parameter of another subsystem (cf. Freeman, 1985).
Such a mechanism seems to play a
role in the regulation of
the α-rhythm. The α-rhythm appears
in the
EEG of occipital
brain regions, after
cessation of visual input, either by closure of both eyes or by darkening
the visual environment. It can be blocked abruptly by a flash of light.
Termination of the visual
input corresponds to a sort
of functional deafferentiation of
the visual cortex. The
α-rhythm apparently corresponds to the 'intrinsic' mode of behaviour of the
visual cortex, which is disturbed by input from 'lower' visual centers. The
output of these centers
acts as control
parameter for
the visual
cortex. The 'intrinsic' behaviour (α-rhythm)
has a much lower fractal dimension then the fast erratic background EEG
seen
in the presence of visual input.
A similar mechanism seems to be operating during sleep-wakefulness transitions. Sleep is not a single physiological state, but instead consists of a sequence of stages, which differ in EEG activity and levels of vigilance. A common denominator seems to be a low level of sensory information processing. Focusing on EEG activity, deep sleep is characterized by a relatively high amplitude and abundance of lower frequencies, indicating a high degree of synchronization. When sleep becomes deeper, the degree of synchronization increases. In addition, there are periods with a fast, small amplitude EEG, which is difficult to distinguish from the waking EEG. In this stage rapid eye movements occur, giving rise to the term Rapid Eye Movement sleep. REM sleep is characterized by desynchronized EEG activity and a low muscular tone (paralysis). Four stages of non-REM sleep are distinguished, stage 3 and 4 corresponding to deep sleep. Babloyantz et al. (1985) presented evidence for chaotic dynamics of brain activity in the human EEG during sleep. Analyzing EEGs using recently developed techniques, they were able to estimate the fractal dimension for sleep stages 2 and 4. For neither REM sleep nor waking EEGs were they able to prove the existence of a chaotic attractor. Dimensionality was much higher in these cases. The increase in synchronization seen in non-REM sleep when going from stage 1 to 4 is paralleled by a decrease in dimensionality and an increase in coherence.
Non-REM sleep is also accompanied by a general reduction in metabolic
activity as measured by the14C-deoxyglucose method (Kennedy
et al., 1982).
Moreover, characteristic dark markings (periodic spatial variations of
metabolic activity) which were observed in nearly all major subdivisions of
the cortex of waking animals, disappeared during sleep. In the same study,
Kennedy et al. (1982) investigated
75 brain structures, in search for a structure in which activity would be
selectively increased during non-REM sleep. However, in all structures
glucose utilization dropped during non-REM sleep. These findings suggest
that non-REM sleep and wakefulness are different functional states of the
brain, which are intrinsic. Transitions between them are characterized by
changes in the level of dissipation.
Non-REM sleep could result from functional deafferentiation of the
cortex. Deep structures, like the reticular formation, which are thought to
be involved in regulation of sleep, could produce changes in the level of
synchronization by a mechanism of general activation.
The most dramatic changes in brain activity occur during
epileptic seizures.
During a seizure, normal brain function is suddenly interrupted and the
activity of the whole brain or part of it sharply increases and becomes very
synchronized and nearly periodic. A recent classification (cf.
Delgado-Escueta et al., 1986)
distinguishes generalized epilepsies where the whole brain is involved in a
seizure, from partial
(a)
epilepsies where the epileptogenic activity stays localized in a certain region. A
second subdivision is made in
idiopathic epilepsies (which are genetically determined) and
symptomatic epilepsies (which are
caused by a structural lesion). Idiopathic generalized epilepsies are least
understood, both because no structural deficits can be shown and because the
epileptogenic activity appears suddenly in several brain structures at
once. No epileptogenic
focus can be found which is primarily responsible for the
onset of the seizures, as
is the case for symptomatic epilepsies. For none of the epilepsies the
primary cause has been established.
Various animal and in vitro models are used to study the mechanisms involved. These
investigations led to a variety of hypotheses of processes and compounds
which could be of primary importance for seizure onset (Delgado-Escueta
et al., 1986). The list of putative primary causes grows steadily and seems to
keep in pace with advances in basic research. Various drugs with widely
different mechanisms of action are known to promote convulsions. These
observations lead to the conclusion that the same pathological state of the
brain can be arrived at in a variety of ways. The sudden and unpredictable
transition
from normal brain function to epileptogenic behaviour seems to be universal
and not dependent on primary cause. It
is proposed that a seizure corresponds to an intrinsic mode of behaviour
where activity is highly synchronized, but unlike non-REM sleep stages, the
level of activity is extremely high. Both excitatory and inhibitory neuron
pools are highly activated. Negative feedback connections prevent
simultaneous strong activation of both pools, but during a seizure periods
of strong excitatory activity are alternated with period of strong
inhibitory activity, giving rise to the characteristic periodic peak-wave
complexes. That this seizure state also exists in non-epileptic subjects is
shown by the results of electro-shock treatment, where the therapeutic
effects are due to the seizure which results from it. In non-epileptic
subjects, this seizure state can only be reached after a very strongly
synchronizing stimulus like an electro-shock or with the aid of convulsants.
Normal brain function is characterized by a subtle balance in the levels of
activity of excitatory and inhibitory neuron pools, allowing them to be
simultaneously activated at a relatively moderate level. This gives rise to
chaotic modes of behaviour on a variety of attractors with different
dimensionality. Disturbance of this balance by a variety of causes can lead
to the onset of a seizure, which corresponds either to a limit cycle
attractor or a chaotic attractor of very low dimensionality. Babloyantz and
Destexhe (1986) analyzed an EEG recording obtained during an absence (petit
mal seizure) and found evidence for a chaotic attractor
with a
very low
fractal dimension. Freeman (1986)
showed that
a seizure can result from runaway inhibition, thereby challenging
the general view that epileptic activity always results from excessive
excitation or failure of inhibition. In the same paper, Freeman also
suggests a chaotic nature of seizure
activity.
The above observations seem to suggest that the brain operates in a chaotic
mode. Different functional states correspond to different strange
attractors, the properties of which still need to be classified. During
non-REM sleep stages bifurcations to low dimensional strange attractors take
place, as a result of a drop in overall activity. In addition to this set of
attractors with relatively high dimensionality and complexity, there exists
at least one very low dimensional strange attractor, which corresponds to an
epileptic mode of behaviour. Transitions to this attractor take place after
one of the other attractors has become unstable. As a result, this
transition can be abrupt and generalized. Probably a variety of causes can
lead to the development of and instability of the high dimensional
attractors governing normal brain function. Once a proper instability has
developed, a bifurcation to a seizure state will result. The behaviour in this state does not
depend on the initial cause, but will be universal.
Does a chaotic mode of operation make sense from a functional point of view?
Chaos provides the opportunity to combine a high complexity (large
dimensionality) with a high degree of organization (reflected by the fractal
structure of the strange attractors). The high level of dissipation in the
brain assures the stability of attractors, so that they can exist for
prolonged periods of time and produce different modes of behaviour.
(a) The term 'partial' epilepsy/seizure has been replaced with 'focal' epilepsy/seizure.